Fire Cues
|
Situational
|
Dynamic
|
More, closer, unexpected and more intense fire cues lead to higher perceived risk
|
Hazard proximity
|
Situational
|
Dynamic
|
Inconclusive
|
Floor level
|
Situational
|
Dynamic
|
The higher the floor, the higher the perceived risk
|
Context
|
Situational
|
Static
|
Inconclusive
|
Credibility of information
|
Situational
|
Static
|
Credibility of information moderates information processing and perceived risk with potential interaction effects of the source of information (another person vs. system)
|
Complexity of information
|
Situational
|
Dynamic
|
Inconclusive
|
Gender
|
Individual
|
Static
|
Tendency toward lower perceived risk in men, but effects are potentially modulated by age and context
|
Age
|
Individual
|
Static
|
Inconclusive
|
Previous experience
|
Individual
|
Static
|
Direct effects of previous experience on perceived risk are inconclusive.
|
Behavioral training
|
Individual
|
Static
|
Inconclusive
|
Hazard knowledge
|
Individual
|
Static
|
Knowledge about hazards increases perceived risk
|
Property attachment
|
Individual
|
Static
|
Inconclusive
|
Personality traits
|
Individual
|
Static
|
Inconclusive
|
Emotional states
|
Individual
|
Dynamic
|
High arousal and state anxiety increase perceived risk
|
Medical factors
|
Individual
|
Dynamic
|
Inconclusive
|
Cognitive abilities
|
Individual
|
Static
|
Inconclusive
|
Information Processing
|
Individual
|
Dynamic
|
Information that is processed easily may be associated with lower perceived risk
|
Trust in authorities
|
Individual
|
Static
|
High trust reduces perceived risk; low trust increases perceived risk
|
Cognitive bias
|
Individual
|
Dynamic
|
Inconclusive
|
Behavior of others
|
Social
|
Dynamic
|
Behavior of others moderates the link between perceived risk and protective action
|
Social roles
|
Social
|
Dynamic
|
Inconclusive
|
Groups
|
Social
|
Dynamic
|
Higher perceived risk in groups
|
Organizational context
|
Organizational
|
Dynamic
|
Inconclusive
|